A University of Melbourne Essay Assignment
Under the 3rd-Year Sociology subject "Social Differences & Inequalities"
Passed with High Distinction (H1)
============ ============ ============
By Benjamin L., written during Semester 1, 2013
Tutor's comments (J. Flores):
This was a very interesting and well-researched essay. You show that you have a thorough understanding of the main issues addressed in this subject. This essay was addressing the complex question of multiculturalism and the reproduction of inequalities in a very sophisticated manner and it was well-structured up until the section 'Cognitive Roots of Discrimination' [cognitive psychology/categorisation/essentialism]. That part did not contribute to the question you had to answer, and while the rest of your essay uses social analyses, this section is unnecessary to your overall argument. It would have been better to engage more with Hage's ideas on tolerance, for example. This essay needs to be more about social factors and while psychology/cognitive makeup may be related, you should focus more on sociology for your research in relation to this subject. This was a very interesting read.
[Please read the author's acknowledgement of the special status of Indigenous Australians with regards to multicultural issues at the end of the essay]
INTRODUCTION
Multiculturalism
aspires towards an inclusive, egalitarian society (Levey, 2009). In Australia,
multiculturalism recognises ethnic diversity and affirms the right to practice
one’s culture within legal boundaries, along with a “reciprocal obligation” to
respect the rights of others to do likewise (National Multicultural Advisory
Council, 1999). These rights are bestowed upon individuals rather than ethnic
groups (Carter, 2006) and avoids charges of essentialism where cultural groups
are mistakenly perceived as internally homogenous (Squires, 2008).
However,
evidence suggests that the practice of cultural identification and
racialisation recognises groups instead. Cultural identification involves
placing others in ethnic groups or populations whom outsiders perceive as
culturally and historically distinct (Smith, 1991). Racialisation is the
process that identifies these groups based on their phenotypic appearance
(‘race’), cultural practices, or other manifested signs of otherness that
become associated with stigmatising stereotypes (Dunn et al., 2007).
In
practice, racialisations are grounded in concrete acts that translate
disapproving attitudes into prejudicial ‘racist’ behaviours (ibid.). By
applying the theoretical frameworks of new racism and aversive racism, this
paper argues that multiculturalism facilitates the reproduction of symbolic and
material inequalities along ethnic lines by creating the false impression that
racism is confined to the past. Instead it is rooted in, but is not reducible
to, our cognitive architecture.
New Racism & Patterns of Political Discourse
Dunn
et al. (2007) argues that while ‘old’ racism is based on biology, new racism
rests on narrow constructions of ‘self’ and the ethnic ‘other’ where race
merely serves as a metonymic marker of broader cultural difference. It
constrains the ethnic others’ rights because their culture is deemed morally
objectionable or incompatible with society as defined by a protagonist, who
reinforces his/her cultural primacy by justifying grounded racialisations in
three ways. Firstly, the self and other is placed into unequal hierarchies of inferiority and
superiority. Secondly, forms of social exclusion (differentiations) are devised. Thirdly, these are based on inherentisms where racialised groups are
assumed to have an inherent, stable, and homogenous core that develop into
stereotypes which can be identified and criticized. Race only functions as an
identifier of cultural difference. All three racialisations have been used to
privilege the Anglo ethnic group over others.
Up
until WWII, the Australian island-continent was envisioned as an exclusively
white, Anglo, British territory where non-white peoples, specifically
Aboriginal and Asian, were undesirable elements to be removed (Ang, 2003).
These discrepancies in racial desirability created an ethnic hierarchy with a
White-Anglo apex and became institutionalized as the Immigration Restriction
Act (1901) or White Australia policy (ibid.). It acted as a form of
differentiation that sought to exclude non-Anglo immigrants and was implicitly
premised upon inherentisms about ‘Aboriginal’, ‘Asian’, ‘Anglo’ or even ‘White’
ethnic categories that made them identifiable and distinguishable.
In
the 1970s, the government sought to remove racial discrimination from official
policy and created a new multicultural definition of nationhood where no ethnic
group had preeminence and Anglo-Australians stood alongside other ethnic groups (Moran, 2011). This officially
removed the ethnic hierarchies and differentiations that characterized earlier
policies but their underlying inherentisms went unaddressed and allowed
conservatives to reinvent old forms of cultural inequality. John Howard,
Australia’s second-longest serving Prime Minister (National Archives of
Australia 2012), did not believe in cultural equality and held that Australia
had a distinctively Anglo-Saxon culture which other migrants and cultures had
to adapt to (Commonwealth of Australia, 2006). We can thus see that the same
inherentisms surrounding the ‘Anglo’ ethnic group and, by extension, non-Anglo
ones are rearticulated into a new hierarchy with an old Anglo apex.
However,
these new hierarchies are harder to criticise. Criticisms about symbolic ethnic
inequalities can be defused with reference to the fact that immigrants are
welcomed. Accusations of hierarchies are also discredited as Anglo ethnic
culture is equated with a seemingly national and ethnically neutral
‘Australian’ culture in a multicultural age. In Hage’s (1988) terms, the Anglo
group thus consolidates itself as the disinterested protector of the ‘national
order’, even as it gives itself an elevated status
Racism without Intent: Aversive Racism &
Inequality
Multiculturalism
can also exacerbate material inequalities by masking unconscious prejudicial
acts. Ethnic discrimination is performatively produced (Bhabha, 1994) and only
exist when individuals hold and act upon ideologies of ethno-racial prejudice
(Luke & Carrington, 2000). Multiculturalism opposes such prejudice, but
theorists of aversive racism argue that unconscious prejudicial and discriminatory
behaviour can still exist despite conscious
multicultural beliefs.
Gaertner
& Dovidio (2005) defines aversive racism as the presence of dissociated
explicit and implicit attitudes about racial groups and their associated
stereotypes. Aversive racists explicitly and consciously cherish egalitarian
values and racial equality and will deny any accusations of racism. However,
they simultaneously hold implicit and
negative biases towards racial others. These implicit attitudes result in
feelings of aversion when the racial other is physically or symbolically
present and suggests that the other has been identified by signs of otherness
and racialised into a an inherentism or stereotype (Dunn et al., 2007).
Aversive
racism is evident in ‘skill discounting’, where the qualifications of job
applicants are discounted on the basis of implicit racist attitudes but
justified on non-ethnic grounds (Shinnaoui & Narchal, 2010). Skill
discounting has been well-documented in the recruitment and integration of foreign-trained
doctors (Louis et al., 2010). The uncertainty about a doctor’s training in a
given country encourages a stereotypical view of the doctor based on that
country and, in Dunn et al.’s (2007) terms, discrimination against the doctor
is implicitly motivated by racialisation but overtly justified with reference
to his dubious training despite
his/her professional recognition in Australia. This skill discounting may in
turn affect the doctor’s prospects by limiting his patient base or his
employer’s perception of his competence (Louis et al., 2007).
Multiculturalism
disguises aversive racism by implying that those who explicitly hold
multicultural beliefs are not racist even though racism occurs despite such
beliefs. It creates the false impression that overt racism is confined to
Australia’s past (Carter, 2006) when it has only been replaced by aversive racism. This jeopardises migrants’ prospects
and puts their economic and mental well-being at risk (Shinnaoui & Narchal,
2010). Seen this way, multiculturalism can blind us to newer, less obvious
forms of racism that mask the reproduction of material inequalities along
ethnic lines.
Blindsided: Inter-Minority Racism
We
have seen how racialisations and inherentisms underpin the reproduction of
cultural and material inequalities. In Australia, this has historically
occurred along majority-minority lines and most studies on ethnic prejudice
worldwide have also limited their focus to this dimension (Weitzer, 1997).
However, minorities can also engage in racialisations and inherentisms against
both the majority (Perlmutter, 2002; Shelton, 2006) and other minorities
(Kohatsu et al, 2011). They may also be employers, politicians, and other
persons of power just as ‘white’ individuals are, and there is emerging evidence
that the historical habit of viewing multiculturalism along white-nonwhite
lines may obscure inter-minority prejudice and inequality.
Barlow
et al. (2010) found that self-identified Asian Australians who felt socially
rejected by those of Aboriginal descent experienced more anxiety during
inter-ethnic encounters (inter-group anxiety) and this led to implicit cultural
prejudice towards Aboriginal Australians in general. Barlow et al. extrapolated
that this would make Aboriginal individuals feel that Asian Australians
rejected them, which would similarly induce intergroup anxiety and cultural
prejudice against Asian Australians in general. This vicious cycle has
implications for the reproduction of cultural and material inequality. Barlow
et al.’s psychometric measures include items like “Many ethnic groups have come
to Australia and worked their way up, and Aboriginal Australians should do the
same without any special favour” (ibid. p. 809). Those who strongly identified
as Asian Australian ranked high in such implicit measures and also disapproved
of a national apology for the hardship inflicted upon Aboriginal communities
throughout Australia’s history. However, some minorities are more disadvantaged
than others (ibid.) and especially true with Aboriginal groups that have long
suffered stolen lands, Stolen Generations, and Stolen Wages (Kidd, 2003). For
example, the notion that Aborigines should better themselves without assistance
is consistent with stereotypes that they are lazy, refuse work, and receive too
much welfare leeching off others (Korff, 2012). These inherentisms then
encourage racial discrimination in Aboriginal employment (Council for
Aboriginal Reconciliation, 2012). The largely white-nonwhite focus of
multicultural discourse in Australia can thus blind us to how such
inter-minority prejudices may exacerbate the same cultural and material
inequalities that multiculturalism aspires to prevent.[1]
The Cognitive Roots of Discrimination
The
common theme behind all the above is the pervasiveness of inherentisms in
prejudice, how racialisations are used to place others into ethnic categories,
and how these underpin various hierarchies and differentiations. The
applicability of this process regardless of historical periods, multicultural
beliefs, or minority status suggests that a more fundamental cognitive
mechanism is at work: psychological essentialism.
In
presuming that ethnic categories have an inherent, stable, and homogenous core,
inherentisms are psychological
essentialisms – the belief that all members of a category share a common
essence that causes their observable features (Prentice & Miller, 2007).
These categories are mentally represented as natural and stable across time,
have definitive boundaries separating it from other categories, are responsible
for the category’s observable features, and category membership is involuntary
and immutable. Ethnicity, along with race, gender, and physical disability, are
especially susceptible to essentialisation (ibid.).
According
to Gil-White (2001) these types of categories are called “nature-kinds” and are
analogous to how we categorise natural objects based on properties that suggest
the presence of some fundamental essence common to all members of a category.
If the object is perceived to possess the essence, this will be sufficient for
category inclusion despite other variations in appearance. While the process of
identifying someone correctly is probabilistic due to the manifold ways a
person might appear and behave, the categories in which they are placed are
clear-cut. Gil-White likens this to the story of the ugly duckling: the fabled
ugly duckling was thought to be a duck based on the essence of descent from
mother-duck, but this was later revised when the ugly-duckling was clearly a
swan. The placing of the ugly
duckling was probabilistic, but the categories
of duck and swan were always clear.
The
prejudice-laden story of the ‘ugly duckling’ can be applied to ugly human
histories. When selectively attend to one dimension of an object, the perceived
difference between objects becomes exaggerated. Nosofsky’s (1986) study on
categorization presents a situation where cognitive objects vary by colour,
shape, and size. As shown in panel A of figure 1 below, the objects are
semantically equidistant from one another as indicated by their connecting
lines.
Figure 1. How selective attention influences
stimulus similarity (Nosofsky, 1986)
In panel B, selectively attending to one dimension,
colour, creates a category with an essence that exaggerates inter-categorical
differences and intra-categorical homogeneity. Similarly, selectively focusing
on a ‘colour’ dimension during social encounters exaggerates the difference
between people’s skin colour, national colour, religious colour and so on.
Furthermore, evidence suggests that essentialism is positively correlated with
negative stereotypes in social categories across cultures and emerges early in
childhood with minimal social input (Deeb et al., 2011). It is these cognitive
essentialisms or inherentisms that underpin the racialisation process and the
prejudicial hierarchies and differentiations that follow. Thus while
multicultural themes of acceptance gain ‘anti-racist’ overtones through its
opposition to rejection (Hage, 1988), racism is not dead in the catacombs of
history. We carry its dormant progeny in our cognitive architecture where it
cannot die, but only be made inert.
CONCLUSION
In
conclusion, multiculturalism facilitates the reproduction of symbolic and
material inequalities along ethnic lines by creating the false impression that
racism is confined to the past. Firstly, while political discourse after White
Australia was characterised by multicultural aspirations, it was also
interspersed with conservative articulations of Anglo cultural supremacy and
the symbolic devaluation of other cultures. Conservatives like PM John Howard
asserted the primacy of Anglo-Saxon culture as a national ‘Australian’ culture
while official multiculturalism and ethnically-neutral immigration policies
covered up a self-serving cultural ideology.
Secondly,
aversive racism has emerged at the micro-social level where essentialisms
inform prejudicial behaviours that adversely affect ethnic others. As shown in
skill discounting, adversive racism can negatively impact one’s right to an
equality of opportunity. This unconscious form of racism suggests that
multiculturalism must go beyond multicultural beliefs and performatively ‘walk
the talk’.
Thirdly,
the historical emphasis on racism between the white majority and non-white
minorities distracts us from the fact that it also exists between ethnic
minorities. This has important implications on social equality as members of
minority groups acquire managerial positions within the economy and the state
where their beliefs actions have wide-ranging consequences. This area needs
attention: Muslim and Arab groups are the least tolerated by members of all
other ethnic groups (Dandy & Pe-Pua, 2010) and ‘Islamaphobia’ is on the rise
in a post-9/11 world (Dunn et al., 2004).
Finally,
the essentialistic inherentisms that prejudice stems from is rooted in our
cognitive architecture. The false impression that racism is long past distracts
us from the fact that it lies dormant within us all. Multicultural beliefs and
practices can never kill racism – it can only render it inert. Whether it is
White Australia or ‘neo-white’ Anglo primacy, prejudice stems from the human
predisposition to classify, identify, and essentialise. This may be what
Australia needs to remember most as it attempts to forge a truly multicultural
society where ethnicity is no barrier to equality.
References
Ang, J. (2003) From White Australia
to Fortress Australia: The anxious nation in the new century, in Jayasuriya L.,
Walker D. and Gothard J. (eds.) Legacies of White Australia: Race, Culture
and Nation. Perth:
University of Western Australia Press.
Australian Council for Aboriginal
Conciliation (2012) Partnerships in Reconciliation. <http://www.austlii.edu.au/au/orgs/car/partner/pbook/page5.htm>.
Barlow, F.K., Louis,
W.R., & Terry, D.J. (2010) 'Minority report: Social identity, cognitions of
rejection, and intergroup anxiety predicting prejudice form one racially
marginalized group towards another', European Journal of Social Pscychology,
Vol. 40, pp. 805-818.
Bhabha, H. (1997) ‘Introduction’ in The
Location of Culture. London: Routledge.
Carter, D. (2006) Disposession,
Dreams & Diversity: Issues in Australian studies. NSW: Pearson
Education.
Commonwealth of Australia (2006) Transcript
of the Prime Minister the Hon John Howard MP Interview with Neil Mitchell,
Radio 3AW, Melbourne. Viewed 22 March 2013, <http://pandora.nla.gov.au/pan/10052/20060321-0000/www.pm.gov.au/news/interviews/Interview1788.html>.
Dandy, J. & Pe-Pua, R. (2010)
'Attitudes to multiculturalism, immigration and cultural diversity: Comparison
of dominant and non-dominant groups in three Australian states'. International
Journal of Intercultural Relations, Vol. 34, pp. 34-46.
Debb, I., Segall, G., Birnbaum, D.,
Ben-Eliyahu, A., & Diesendruck, G. (2011) 'Seeing isn't believing: The
effect of intergroup exposure on children's essentialist beliefs about ethnic
categories'. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, pp. 1-18.
Dunn, K.M., Forrest, J., Burnley,
I., McDonald, A. (2004) 'Constructing racism in Australia'. Australian
Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 39, No. 4, pp. 409-430.
Dunn, K.M., Klocker,
N., & Salabay, T. (2007) 'Contemporary racism and Islamaphobia in
Australia: Racializing religion'. Ethnicities, Vol. 7, pp. 564-589.
Gaertner S.L. & Dovidio J.F.
(2005) 'Understanding and addressing contemporary racism: From aversive racism
to the common identity in-group model'. Journal of Social Issues, Vol.
61, No. 3, pp. 615-639.
Gil-White, F.J. (2001) 'Are ethnic
groups biological "species" to the human brain?'. Current
Anthropology, Vol. 42, No. 4, pp. 515-554.
Hage, G. (1988) White Nation:
Fantasies of White supremacy in a multicultural society. Armadale: Pluto Press.
Kohatsu, E.L., Victoria, R., Andrew,
L., Michelle, F., & Salazar, A. (2011) 'Analyzing anti-Asian prejudice from
a racial identity and color-blind perspective'. Journal of Counseling &
Development, Vol. 89, pp. 63-72.
Kidd, R. (2003) Much more than money: The fight to recover the Stolen Wages. International
Human Rights' Day Symposium, Centre for Public Culture and Ideas, Griffith
University, viewed 26 March 2013,
<http://www.linksdisk.com/roskidd/tpages/t18.htm>.
Korff, J. (2012) 'Stereotypes &
prejudice of 'Aboriginal People', Creative Spirits. Viewed 25 March
2012, <http://pandora.nla.gov.au/pan/130299/20121215-0001/CreativeSpirits/www.creativespirits.info/aboriginalculture/people/stereotypes-prejudice-of-aboriginal-australia>.
Levey, G.B. (2009) 'What is living
and what is dead in multiculturalism'. Ethnicities, Vol. 9, No. 1, pp.
75-93.
Louis, W.R., Lalonde, R.N., &
Esses, V.M. (2010) 'Bias against foreign-born or foreign-trained doctors:
Experimental evidence'. Medical Education, Vol. 44, pp. 1241-1247.
Luke, C. and Carrington, V., (2000)
'Race matters'. Journal of Intercultural Studies, Vol. 21, No. 1, pp.
213-232.
Moran, A. (2011) 'Multiculturalism
as nation-building in Australia: Inclusive national identity and the embrace of
diversity'. Ethnic and Racial Studies, Vol. 34, No. 1, pp. 2153-2172.
National Archives of Australia
(2012) John Howard. Viewed 22 March 2013, <http://primeministers.naa.gov.au/primeministers/howard/>
National Multicultural Advisory
Council (1999) Australian Multiculturalism for a New Century: Towards
inclusiveness. Commonwealth of Australia: Canberra.
Nosofsky, R.M. (1986) 'Attention,
similarity, and the Identification-Categorization Relationship'. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, Vol. 115, No. 1, pp. 39-57.
Perlmutter, P. (2002)
' Minority group prejudice'. Society, Vol. 39, No. 3, pp. 59-65.
Prentice, D.A. & Miller, D.T.
(2007) 'Psychological essentialism of human categories'. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, Vol. 16, No. 4, pp. 202-206.
Shelton, J.N. & Richeson, J.A.
(2006) 'Ethnic minorities' racial attitudes and contact experiences with white
people'. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, Vol. 12, No.
1, pp. 149-164.
Shinnaoui, D. & Narchal, R.
(2010) 'Brain gain to brain waste: Individual biases, prejudice, and
discounting of migrant skills'. International Migration & Integration,
Vol. 11, pp. 423-437.
Smith, A. D., (1991) ‘The ethnic
basis of national identity’ in National Identity. London: Penguin.
Squires, J. (2008)
'Multiculturalism, multiple groups and inequalities'. Social & Legal
Studies, Vol. 17, pp. 535-542.
Weitzer, R. (1997) 'Racial prejudice among Korean
merchants in African American neighborhoods'. The Sociological Quaterly,
Vol. 3, No. 4, pp. 587-606.
[1] The author wishes to acknowledge that Aboriginal communities may
have deep spiritual ties to the land and that this may not be suitably
respected by placing them as one-of-many ethnic groups under a multicultural
arrangement. This is best explored by other, more specific studies.
0 comments:
Post a Comment