A University of Melbourne APA Short Lab Report Assignment
Under the 2nd-Year Psychology subject "Cognitive Psychology"
Passed with High Distinction (H1)
============ ============ ============
By Benjamin L., written during Semester 1, 2012
Abstract
In Feature
Integration Theory, a ‘master map’ facilitates visual search by highlighting
objects that contain the target’s features and directs subsequent conjunction
search towards these few candidates. This suggests that the master map would be
ineffective when targets and distractors share identical features. Under these
conditions, this study hypothesised that the master map would be ineffective
and conjunction search accuracy would decrease as set size increased. 35 participants
performed self-terminating visual search tasks for conjunction targets amidst
varying numbers of distractors. Results showed that search accuracy decreased
with greater set size. This suggests that the master map is ineffective when
target and distractors share identical features.
Tutor's comments (J. Chan)[not Jackie Chan]:
Overall, excellent work Ben - this is a well formulated lab report! Well done on integrating recent research seamlessly! [Note: For this assignment, we had to focus on Feature Integration Theory rather than more recent accounts]
Feature Integration
Theory: Master Map Inefficacy and Conjunction Search
Visual
search involves perceiving both an object’s constituent features and their
combination (Prinzmetal, 1995). Single features are rarely sufficient for finding
objects (Dehaene, 1989) but simultaneously processing all the visual stimuli in
everyday life is impossible (Wolfe & Horowitz, 2004). Instead, visual
attention mechanisms process most stimuli at a superficial level, while only a
few objects undergo extensive analysis. This allows us to perform the many
everyday tasks that require visual search (Wolfe, 1993).
One
influential account is Feature Integration Theory (FIT) (Wolfe & Horowitz,
2004). In FIT, stimuli are coded based on distinct dimensions like colour,
orientation, and size (Treisman & Gelade, 1980). Dimensions are categories
of features that objects may possess, while features are variations within a
dimension. For example, the dimension ‘colour’ contains the feature ‘redness’. Focal
attention is essential for conjoining these features into a unitary object (Groeger,
Clegg, & O’Shea, 2005) and individuals with impaired visual attention are
unable to do this (Arguin, Joanette, & Cavanagh, 1993).
Feature
searches locate targets that are distinguished by a single perceptual dimension
(Chan & Hayward, 2009). Features are processed automatically,
pre-attentively, and in parallel across vision (Quinlan, 2003), making feature
search efficient and independent of the number of stimuli present (set size) (Treisman
& Gelade, 1980). Conjunction searches locate targets that are distinguished
by conjunctions of multiple features (Wolfe,
1993). As focal attention is required to “glue” features into conjunctions, conjunction
search must serially attend to each possible target in turn (Treisman &
Gelade, 1980). This serial process makes it is less efficient and search times
increase with set size (Chan & Hayward, 2009).
FIT
was challenged by reports of efficient conjunction search and was subsequently revised
to include a “master map” (Chan & Hayward, 2009). A feature search would locate
objects that contained the target’s features and plot their location onto a
“master map”. Next, the master map directs conjunction search solely towards these
few locations, thus resulting in more efficient search times.
However,
the master map would not work if all the objects contain the target’s features.
Since the master map is essentially a feature-based mechanism, visual displays
where both targets and distractors contain identical features should logically prevent
the master map from highlighting possible targets. Subsequent conjunction
search would have to scan objects without prior guidance until the target is
found. Given that conjunction search proceeds serially (Treisman & Gelade,
1980), the time taken to locate a target should increase with set size and if
the visual scene is only briefly present, individuals may not have enough time
to accurately locate the target. It is therefore hypothesised that, when targets
and distractors have identical features, the master map is ineffective and conjunction
search accuracy will decrease as set size increases.
Methods
The stimuli were presented on a
projector screen using an overhead projector. 35 observers (24 female, age range 19-25 years old) viewed
the stimuli while seated comfortably in a classroom. Each stimulus was
presented using Microsoftâ
PowerPointâ. The stimulus was presented for
approximately 1 second. The background was white and each stimulus comprised
four white squares with black borders arranged in a 2x2 grid. Within each
square there were either two digits (Condition 1) or twenty-six digits
(Condition 2). The digits were always black. The target (the number “2”) was
always present in one (and only one) square. All the other digits were always
the number “5” (the distractors). Although the angular size of the digits was
not controlled (the observers viewed the stimuli from different distances), the
digits were always large enough to be easily readable.
The numbers “2” and “5” were
constructed so that they contained exactly the same line segments (two
horizontal line segments and three vertical line segments), albeit arranged
differently. Please see Figures 1 and 2.
The observer’s task was to identify
which square contained the number 2 (the target). Each square was uniquely labeled with a letter from the
following set {A, B, C, D}. At the end of each trial, the observer wrote down
which square they thought contained the target using this alphabetical code.
In each condition, 10 trials were
run, alternating between the two conditions. Using an answer sheet, the
observer’s average accuracy in each condition was determined. This data was
then collapsed across observers and a paired t-test was performed on the
resultant data using the software package SPSSâ.
Figure 2: An example of
the stimulus used in Condition 2
Results
In
this study, the independent variable was set size and the dependent variable
was visual search accuracy in terms of the percentage of correct responses.
Results are represented in figure 1 below.
Figure
1. Mean Accuracy in Condition 1 & 2
with 95% confidence intervals
In
figure 1, participants in condition 1 had a mean accuracy of 88.85 (SD = 5.29) while
those in condition 2 had a mean accuracy of 53.42 (SD = 16.79). A repeated
measures t-test revealed that these means were significantly different (t(34) = 13.281, p < 0.001). These
results showed that conjunction search in condition 2, which had a larger set
size, was significantly less accurate compared to condition 1, which had a
smaller set size.
Discussion
The
results above support the hypothesis that, when target and distractor features
are identical, the master map is ineffective and conjunction search accuracy decreases
as set size increases. This is consistent with previous research which found
that conjunction search times increased with set size (Treisman & Gelade,
1980; Dehaene, 1989), thus briefly presented visual displays with large set
sizes (i.e. condition 2) may not give participants enough time to correctly locate
the target and result in less accurate responses. Conversely, a small set size
presented for the same, brief duration (i.e. condition 1) would result in more
accurate responses.
This
coheres with descriptions of the master map and conjunction search in FIT.
Since the target and distractors in this study consist of identical features –
comprising of three horizontal lines and two vertical lines – the ‘2’ target is
only distinguishable through a conjunction search that identifies how its
features are conjoined (Wolfe, 1993). However, conjunction search is a serial
process and attention must be focused on each object in turn until the target
is found (Treisman & Gelade, 1980). If the display is only presented
briefly, participants may not have enough time to correctly locate the target
and produce inaccurate responses.
When some - but not all the distractors - share the target’s features, as in this study, the master map cannot facilitate
conjunction search. The map functions by using an automatic, preattentive feature
search to highlight candidate objects
containing the target’s features and and subsequent conjunction search focuses on these few candidates, thus reducing
overall search times (Chan & Hayward, 2009). This pre-selection of
candidates cannot occur when all the distractors contain the target’s features, and
hence the master map will not be able to reduce search times.
However,
this study’s findings are not immediately transferable to naturalistic
settings. The simple character of the stimuli used in this study are useful for
controlled experiments, but everyday visual search tasks often involve more
complex scenes than those in laboratory tests (Wolfe, 1993). Future FIT
research could test if the results of this study are valid with more naturalistic
stimuli.
In
conclusion, this study finds that, when targets and distractors have identical
features, the master map is ineffective and conjunction search accuracy will
decrease as set size increases. Without the master map, conjunction search
remains time-consuming and in a time-scarce situation, this results in
inaccurate responses. Due to the simple character of the stimuli used here,
care must be taken in extending these results beyond theoretical contexts. To
advance our practical understanding of FIT, future research could examine if
these findings still hold when naturalistic stimuli are used instead.
References
Arguin, M., Joanette, Y. &
Cavanagh, P. (1993). Visual search for feature and conjunction targets with an
attention deficit. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 5 (4), 436-452.
Chan, L.K.H. & Hayward, W.G.
(2009). Feature integration theory revisited: Dissociating feature detection
and attentional guidance in visual search. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 35 (1), 119-132.
Dehaene, S. (1989). Discriminability
and dimensionality effects in visual search for featural conjunctions: A
functional pop-out. Perception &
Psychophysics, 46 (1), 72-80.
Groeger, J.A., Clegg, B.A. &
O’Shea, G. (2005). Conjunction search in simulated railway signals: A
cautionary note. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 19, 973-984.
Prinzmetal, W. (1995). Visual
feature integration in a world of objects.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4 (3), 90-94.
Quinlan, P.T. (2003). Visual feature
integration theory: Past, present, and future. Psychological Bulletin, 129 (5), 643-673.
Treisman, A.M. (1980). A
feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive
Psychology, 12, 97-136.
Wolfe, J.M. (1994). Visual search in
continuous, naturalistic stimuli. Vision
Research, 34 (9), 1187-1195.
Wolfe, J.M. & Horowitz, T.S.
(2004). What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do
they do it? Neuroscience, 5, 1-7.
0 comments:
Post a Comment