A University of Short Essay Assignment
Under the Freshman Sociology subject "Critical Analytical Skills"
Passed with Distinction (H2A)
============ ============ ============
By Benjamin L., written during Semester 1, 2012
'[You are] required to find one piece of quantitative social science research and one piece of qualitative social science research (quantitative and qualitative broadly defined) and discuss the qualities of this research in relation to the content from the first 2 weeks of the course'
MULT 20003 – Assessment 1 – Benjamin L. - 396274
Social
science aims to understand the social world as made by people and their actions
within it (Hanacke, 2010 p4). This is achieved by using research methods to
compare theory with reality. (Kalof, Dan & Dietz, 2008). These methods can
be broadly classed as quantitative or qualitative. Quantitative methods are
“variable-oriented” approaches that aim to discern statistical relationships
between variables in large, representative samples or populations (Onwuegbuzie,
Johnson, & Collins, 2009 p4). In contrast, qualitative methods are
“case-oriented” and emphasise the individual, subjective aspects of social life
(ibid). Drawing on one quantitative and one qualitative study, this essay
argues that neither approach is inherently superior. Instead, the chosen must
be suited to the nature of the area under study.
An
example of quantitative research is Nelson, Admanson, & Bakeman’s (2008)
study on theory of mind. They explored the relationship
between parent-toddler joint engagement and its effect on toddlers’ acquisition
of ‘theory of mind’ over time. Nelson and colleagues hypothesised, “joint
engagement experience likely facilitates theory of mind development”. Theory of mind (TOM) is the awareness that others are intentional
beings whose behaviour is guided by mental states that may contradict reality (McAlister
& Peterson, 2007 p1), while joint engagement is the amount of time toddlers
and mothers simultaneously attend to an object, as measured in a controlled
laboratory playroom. The presence of TOM is tested through a ‘false belief
test’, which assesses if toddlers are aware of others’ potentially mistaken
beliefs about a specific object/situation (ibid.).
In
contrast, Luke & Carrington’s (2000) study on interracial marriage is an
example of qualitative research. They interviewed 50 interracial marriages to
explore the ‘lived experience’ [sic] of interracial relationships and families.
Luke & Carrington argue that ‘race’ is a fundamental principle of social
organisation. It shapes public and private life. Couples crossing the racial
‘colour line’ tended to meet with social ‘estrangements’ – disapproval and
rejection from friends, family and strangers – from people of either partner’s
race. The couple eventually made new friendships with other interracial
families and formed a diaspora of “’third space’ otherness” (Appadurai, 1998 in
ibid.) that transcends simple in-group/out-group categories of racial
identitiy.
These
two studies differ in many respects. Firstly, the quantitative study uses a
deductive process while the qualitative study uses an inductive process. The
deductive process involves a hypothetical relationship between two or more
concepts and test them using quantifiable measures (Gray, 2009). Nelson and
colleagues (2008) began with a hypothesis using the precisely defined concepts
of “joint engagement” and “theory of mind”, as mentioned above. They tested
this by quantifying “joint engagement” in seconds while “theory of mind” was
measured by the child’s age, in months, at which they could successfully perform
a false belief test. After averaging toddlers’ scores, the study concluded that
joint engagement significantly influenced TOM development. Their research thus
progresses from theory and hypothesis to quantitative measures before finally
reaching a conclusion.
Quantitative
methods are epistemologically premised on the elimination erroneous and
idiosyncratic elements through statistical averages. From this, we can discover
the “regularity of social processes” and find the ‘average man’, or quintessential
persona, that is representative of a sample group or population (Quetlet, 1831
in Coven, 2003). This stems from a broader positivist epistemological position,
which holds that the social world operated according to a set of strict,
discoverable causal laws (Gray, set 2 p.19). The
strength of quantitative approaches lies in its specificity. The independent
variable (joint engagement) is hypothesised to affect the dependent variable (TOM
acquisition) as a consequence. Each variable is operationalised in
quantitatively measurable form that tells us what changes, and crucially how
much it changes. However, this assumes such causal laws exist beyond the
sample tested. While TOM has been found in toddlers across different countries
(McAlister & Peterson, 2007), the assumption of discoverable laws may not
apply to other areas of social science.
One
such area is Luke and Carrington’s (2000) qualitative study, which follows an
inductive process instead. The inductive process starts with the collection of
data that is then examined for patterns that may imply a relationship between
variables. Luke & Carrington named their area of research – racial concepts
and interracial couples – but do not hypothesise about relationships between
variables. Instead they “draw on interview data from 50 interracial families”
before identifying “precursors to
out-marriage”, and the resulting ‘estrangements’ from others. Data was
qualitative, consisting of couples’ opinions and experiences as they responded
to disapproval and estrangement.
This
research method is based on a constructionist view of the social world, which
holds that multiple but equally valid accounts of the same phenomenon can exist
(Onwuegbuzie, Johnson, & Collins, 2009). It stems from Max Weber’s belief
that social reality consists of individuals who create cultures and values (Hancke, 2010). Humans shaped society
through subjective understanding rather than ‘objective’ causal laws (ibid.).
Indeed, the broad spectrum on social contexts makes precise prediction difficult
(Ekstrom, 1992). Luke & Carrington note racially motivated discrimination
or ‘racisms’ become social reality only when people hold and act on “ideologies
of race differentiation”. As a result, interracial couples were rejected by
both partners’ racial milieu and experienced “complex and unpredictable” events
of “’third space’ otherness”. Since the processes of social phenomena are
fundamentally rooted in subjective cognitions, interviews are better suited for
understanding the spectrum of thoughts underlying social phenomena than
statistical quantification, ‘counts’, or ‘aggregations’ of specific beliefs.
In
conclusion, the choice between quantitative and qualitative methods depends on
the nature of the area under study. Quantitative methods are rooted in a
positivist epistemology that believes strict, independent, and discoverable
laws exist (Gray, 2009). These methods seek statistical generalisations from
representative samples (Coven, 2009) and, with the appropriate measures and
standardised tests, provide precise data that describes a relationship between
variables. Qualitative methods focus on subjective beliefs rather than nomothetical
generalisations (Onwuegbuzie, Johnson, & Collins, 2009). It is rooted in a
constructionist epistemology that admits of multiple valid accounts of the
social world (ibid) and suitably captures “lived experience” at the level of
everyday life rather than abstract theories. While the positivist idea of a
law-governed reality contradicts constructionist notions of multiple subjective
social worlds, the methods derived from either can produce insightful findings
when used on appropriate areas of the social world.
References
Coven, V
2003, A history of statistics in the social sciences, Gateway,
Spring, viewed 10 March 2012,
Ekstrom,
M 1992, 'Causal explanation of social action', Acta Sociologica, vol.
35, pp. 107-122.
Gray, R 2009,
Doing Research in the Real World (second edition), Sage, Los Angeles.
Hancke,
B 2010, 'The challenge of research design', In D Marsh and G Stoker (eds), Theory
and Methods in Political Science (third edition), Palgrave Macmillan, New
York, pp. 232-248.
Kalof,
L, Amy D & Thomas, D 2008, Essentials in Social Research, Open
University Press, Oxford.
Luke, C & Carrington, V 2000,
'Race matters', Journal of Intercultural Studies, vol. 21 no. 1, pp. 5-24.
McAlister, A & Paterson, C 2007,
'A longitudinal study of child siblings and theory of mind development', Cognitive
Development, vol. 22, pp. 258-270.
Nelson, PB, Adamson, LB & Bakeman,
R 2008, 'Toddlers’ joint engagement experience facilitates preschoolers’
acquisition of theory of mind', Developmental Science, vol. 11 no. 6,
pp. 847-852.
Onwuegbuzie, AJ, Johnson, RB, &
Collins, KM 2009, 'Call for mixed analysis: A philosophical framework for
combining qualitative and quantitative approaches', International Journal of
Multiple Research Approaches, vol. 3 pp. 114–139.
0 comments:
Post a Comment